Quantitative Linguistics
Quantitative Linguistics is a
sub-discipline of general linguistics and, more specifically, of mathematical
linguistics. Quantitative Linguistics (QL) deals with language learning,
language change, and application as well as structure of natural languages. QL
investigates languages using statistical methods; its most demanding objective
is the formulation of language laws and, ultimately, of a general theory of
language in the sense of a set of interrelated languages laws[1] Synergetic
linguistics was from its very beginning specifically designed for this
purpose.[2] QL is empirically based on the results of language statistics, a
field which can be interpreted as statistics of languages or as statistics of
any linguistic object. This field is not necessarily connected to substantial
theoretical ambitions. Corpus linguistics and computational linguistics are
other fields which contribute important empirical evidence.
Linguistics is the scientific study of human language.[1][2][3][4] Linguistics can be broadly broken
into three categories or subfields: the study of language form, of language
meaning, and of language in context.
The
first is the study of language structure, or grammar. This focuses on the system of rules followed by the
speakers (or hearers) of a language. It encompasses morphology
(the formation and composition of words), syntax (the formation and composition of phrases and sentences
from these words), and phonology (sound systems). Phonetics is a related branch of linguistics concerned with the
actual properties of speech sounds, nonspeech sounds, and how they are produced
and perceived.
The
study of language meaning
is concerned with how languages employ logical structures and real-world
references to convey, process and assign meaning, as well as to manage and
resolve ambiguity. This subfield encompasses semantics (how meaning is inferred from words and concepts) and pragmatics (how meaning is inferred from context).
Language
in its broader context includes evolutionary linguistics, which considers the origins of
language; historical linguistics, which explores language change; sociolinguistics, which looks at the relation
between linguistic variation and social structures; psycholinguistics, which explores the representation
and function of language in the mind; neurolinguistics, which looks at language processing
in the brain; language acquisition, how children or adults acquire
language; and discourse analysis, which involves the structure of
texts and conversations.
Although
linguistics is the scientific study of language, a number of other intellectual
disciplines are relevant to language and influence its study. Semiotics, for example, is the general study of signs and symbols
both within language and without. Literary theorists study the use of language in literature. Linguistics additionally draws on work from such diverse
fields as psychology, speech-language pathology, informatics,
computer science, philosophy, biology, human anatomy, neuroscience, sociology, anthropology, and acoustics.
Terminology for the discipline
Before
the 20th century, the term "philology", first attested in 1716 was commonly used to refer to the science
of language, which was then predominantly historical in focus.Since Ferdinand de Saussure's insistence on the importance of synchronic analysis, however, this focus has shifted and the term "philology" is now
generally used for the "study of a language's grammar, history and
literary tradition", especially in the United States,where it was never as
popular as it was elsewhere (in the sense of the "science of
language").
Although
the term "linguist" in the sense of "a student of language"
dates from 1641, the term
"linguistics" is first attested in 1847. It is now the usual academic term in
English for the scientific study of language.
The
term linguist, used for one who studies language, applies within the
field to someone who either studies linguistics or uses linguistic
methodologies to study groups of languages or particular languages. Outside the
field, this term is commonly used to refer to people who speak many languages
fluently.
Fundamental concerns and divisions
Linguistics
concerns itself with describing and explaining the nature of human language.
Fundamental questions include what is universal to language, how language can vary,
and how human beings come to know languages. Linguistic fields can then be
broadly divided into those that distinguish themselves by a focus on linguistic
structure and grammar, and those that distinguish themselves by the
nonlinguistic factors they consider.
Fundamental questions
All
humans achieve competence in whatever language is used around them when growing
up, with apparently little need for explicit conscious instruction (setting
aside extremely pathological cases). Linguists assume that the ability to
acquire and use language is an innate, biologically based potential of modern
human beings, similar to the ability to walk, because nonhumans do not acquire
human language in this way (although many nonhuman animals can learn to respond
to language, or can even be trained to use it to a degree )
There
is no consensus, however, as to the extent of humans' innate potential for
language, or the degree to which such innate abilities are specific to
language. Some theorists claim that there is a very large set of highly
abstract and specific binary settings coded into the human brain; the
combinations of these settings would give rise to every language on the planet.
Other linguists claim that the ability to learn language is a product of
general human cognition. It is, however, generally agreed that there are no
strong genetic differences underlying the differences between languages:
an individual will acquire whatever language(s) he or she is exposed to as a
child, regardless of parentage or ethnic origin. Nevertheless, recent research
suggests that even weak genetic biases in speakers may, over a number of
generations, influence the evolution of particular languages, leading to a
nonrandom distribution of certain linguistic features across the world.
Sub-fields
of structure-focused linguistics include:
- Phonetics, the study of the physical properties of speech (or signed) production and perception
- Phonology, the study of sounds (or signs) as discrete, abstract elements in the speaker's mind that distinguish meaning
- Morphology, the study of internal structures of words and how they can be modified
- Syntax, the study of how words combine to form grammatical sentences
- Semantics, the study of the meaning of words (lexical semantics) and fixed word combinations (phraseology), and how these combine to form the meanings of sentences
- Pragmatics, the study of how utterances are used in communicative acts, and the role played by context and nonlinguistic knowledge in the transmission of meaning
- Discourse analysis, the analysis of language use in texts (spoken, written, or signed)
Many linguists would agree that
these divisions overlap considerably, and the independent significance of each
of these areas is not universally acknowledged. Regardless of any particular
linguist's position, each area has core concepts that foster significant
scholarly inquiry and research.
Divisions based on nonlinguistic
factors studied
Alongside
the structurally motivated domains of study are other fields of linguistics.
These fields are distinguished by the kinds of nonlinguistic factors that they
consider:
- Applied linguistics, the study of language-related issues applied in everyday life, notably language policies, planning, and education. (Constructed language fits under Applied linguistics.)
- Biolinguistics, the study of natural as well as human-taught communication systems in animals, compared to human language.
- Clinical linguistics, the application of linguistic theory to the field of Speech-Language Pathology.
- Computational linguistics, the study of computational implementations of linguistic structures.
- Developmental linguistics, the study of the development of linguistic ability in individuals, particularly the acquisition of language in childhood.
- Evolutionary linguistics, the study of the origin and subsequent development of language by the human species.
- Historical linguistics or diachronic linguistics, the study of language change over time.
- Language geography, the study of the geographical distribution of languages and linguistic features.
- Linguistic typology, the study of the common properties of diverse unrelated languages, properties that may, given sufficient attestation, be assumed to be innate to human language capacity.
- Neurolinguistics, the study of the structures in the human brain that underlie grammar and communication.
- Psycholinguistics, the study of the cognitive processes and representations underlying language use.
- Sociolinguistics, the study of variation in language and its relationship with social factors.
- Stylistics, the study of linguistic factors that place a discourse in context.
Semiotics is not a discipline within linguistics; rather, it
investigates the relationship between signs and what they signify more broadly.
From the perspective of semiotics, language can be seen as a sign or symbol,
with the world as its representation.
Historical
linguistics studies the history and evolution of languages through the comparative method. Often the aim of historical
linguistics is to classify languages in language families descending from a common ancestor.
This involves comparison of elements in different languages to detect possible cognates in order to be able to reconstruct
how different languages have changed over time. This also involves the
study of etymology, the study of the history of single
words. Historical linguistics is also called "diachronic linguistics"
and is opposed to "synchronic linguistics" that study languages in a
given moment in time without regarding its previous stages. In universities in
the United States, the historic perspective is often out of fashion. Historical
linguistics was among the first linguistic disciplines to emerge and was the
most widely practiced form of linguistics in the late 19th century. The shift
in focus to a synchronic perspective started with Saussure
and became predominant in western linguistics with Noam Chomsky's emphasis on the study of the synchronic and universal
aspects of language.
Semiotics
Semiotics is the study of sign processes (semiosis), or signification
and communication, signs and symbols, both individually and grouped into sign
systems, including the study of how meaning is constructed and understood.
Semioticians often do not restrict themselves to linguistic communication when
studying the use of signs but extend the meaning of "sign" to cover
all kinds of cultural symbols. Nonetheless semiotic disciplines closely related
to linguistics are literary studies, discourse analysis, text linguistics, and philosophy of language
Descriptive linguistics and language documentation
Since
the inception of the discipline of linguistics
linguists have been concerned with describing and documenting languages
previously unknown to science. Starting with Franz Boas in the early 1900s descriptive linguistics became the main
strand within American linguistics until the rise of formal structural
linguistics in the mid 20th century. The rise of American descriptive
linguistics was caused by the concern with describing the languages of
indigenous peoples that were (and are) rapidly moving towards extinction. The
ethnographic focus of the original Boasian type of descriptive linguistics
occasioned the development of disciplines such as Sociolinguistics, anthropological
linguistics,
and linguistic anthropology, disciplines that investigate the
relations between language, culture and society.
The
emphasis on linguistic description and documentation has since become more
important outside of North America as well, as the documentation of rapidly
dying indigenous languages has become a primary focus in many of the worlds'
linguistics programs. Language description is a work intensive endeavour
usually requiring years of field work for the linguist to learn a language
sufficiently well to write a reference grammar of it. The further task of
language documentation requires the linguist to collect a preferably large
corpus of texts and recordings of sound and video in the language, and to
arrange for its storage in accessible formats in open repositories where it may
be of the best use for further research by other researchers.
Applied linguistics
Linguists
are largely concerned with finding and describing
the generalities and varieties both within particular languages and among all
language. Applied linguistics takes the result of those findings
and "applies" them to other areas. The term "applied
linguistics" is often used to refer to the use of linguistic research in
language teaching only[citation
needed],
but results of linguistic research are used in many other areas as well, such
as lexicography and translation. "Applied linguistics" has been argued to be
something of a misnomer[who?], since applied linguists focus on
making sense of and engineering solutions for real-world linguistic problems,
not simply "applying" existing technical knowledge from linguistics;
moreover, they commonly apply technical knowledge from multiple sources, such
as sociology (e.g. conversation analysis) and anthropology.
References
1. ^ Fromkin, Victoria; Bruce Hayes;
Susan Curtiss, Anna Szabolcsi, Tim Stowell, Donca Steriade
(2000). Linguistics: An Introduction to Linguistic Theory. Oxford:
Blackwell. p. 3. ISBN 0631197117.
2. ^ Martinet, André (1960). Elements of General
Linguistics. Tr. Elisabeth Palmer (Studies in General Linguistics, vol.
i.). London: Faber. p. 15.
3. ^ Halliday, Michael
A. K.; Jonathan
Webster (2006). On Language and Linguistics. Continuum International
Publishing Group. p. vii. ISBN 0826488242.
4. ^ Greenberg, Joseph (1948). "Linguistics and
ethnology". Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 4: 140–47.
6. ^ McMahon, A. M. S. (1994). Understanding
Language Change. Cambridge University Press. p. 19. ISBN 0-521-44665-1
7. ^ McMahon, A. M. S. (1994). Understanding
Language Change. Cambridge University Press. p. 9. ISBN 0-521-44665-1
10. ^ "Linguist". The
American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. Houghton Mifflin
Harcourt. 2000. ISBN 978-0395825174.
12. ^ (Dediu, D. & Ladd,
D.R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the
adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin, PNAS
104:10944-10949;
summary available here)
Komentar
Posting Komentar