Theory Of Anthropology


A theory is a systematic organization of ideas proposed to explain a phenomenon. In other words a theory is simply a speculation, which at the time cannot be proven and is used as a means to explain an event, trend, or other occurrences.
Theories attempt to directly refute well established schools of thought which are widely held as fact. It should be noted that “science [as well as social science] does not rest upon solid bedrock. The bold structure of its theories rises, as it were, above a swamp. It is like a building erected on piles. The piles are driven down from above into the swamp, but not down to any natural or ‘given’ base; and if we stop driving the piles deeper, it is not because we have reached firm ground. We simply stop when we are satisfied that the piles are firm enough to carry the structure, at least for the time being.” (Popper LSD 2002: 122)
In attempting to explain and rationalize the world, Popper defines scientific theories as "universal statements" used to cast a net to "catch" the world. Popper believes our goal is to make the holes in the net as small as possible through modification and revision.(Popper 2002: 37, 38) Consequently, "scientific theories are perpetually changing" (Popper LSD 2002: 50). Furthermore, strict universals are non-verifiable "all-statements" that can not be reduced into a finite number of singular statements, thus they are theories or natural laws.(Popper LSD 2002: 40-42)
Just as science speculates reality of the structure of the universe that we live in, social science comments on the social reality in which we live in. The social reality in which we live in, or as anthropologists, in which we observe, is constantly changing. As post-modernity has progressed, the world has become increasingly globalized, which has created a hybridization of culture. America serves as a paradigmatic example, certain elements of American culture are integrated into other countries of the world. However, discovering what parts of western culture have taken hold in other areas of the world and why cannot be explained in real time, but must be observed then speculated upon. Clearly, not all the contributing factors will be brought to light. By the time many of the contributing factors have been speculated upon the culture will have shifted to a new paradigm.
Theories in social science heavily reflect the time period in which they were written in. Therefore, not all of the premises in the theory which may have been true during the time it was written would still remain so contemporarily. A large problem with social theories is that they are heavily reliant upon the people, while in science an experiment can be conducted over and over again under similar circumstances. Often anthropologists rely on less than reputable members of society which will deceive them for their own ends, as was often the case in early anthropology. Similarly the biases of the anthropologist come into light when analyzing the data gathered. Given that there is no Truth which we can observe, in light of the fact that would take an onyperspective, we are left with creating theories based on our senses on which to constantly revise both in science and social science.
Post-Modern Anthropology is subdivided into 5 distinct categories:
1.      Applied Anthropology
2.      Archeology
4.      Cultural Anthropology

1.      AppliedAnthropology
The practical application of anthropological data, methodology, perspective and theory to asses and solve contemporary social problems.

2.      Archeology
The study of past cultures based on the excavation of their habitation, burial, and environmental sites.

3.      Biological Anthropology
Tracks the biological evolution of humanity through genetic inheritance, primatology and the fossil record.

4.      Cultural Anthropology
The study of populations based on historical records and ethnographic observations. Ethnographies consist of an anthropologist living among another culture participating and observing it as an outsider.


5.      Linguistic Anthropology
Brings linguistic methods as applicable solutions for anthropological problems.
Anthropology Linguistic

Anthropology is the study of man and culture as a whole. On the one hand man is the creator of culture, on the other hand culture "created" human beings with their environment. Thus, the intertwined relationship of reciprocity is very close and compact between the people and culture.
In culture, language occupies a unique place and honorable. Besides as an element of culture, language also serves as an important tool in inheritance, development and dissemination of culture.
Coverage of study related to the very broad language, because language covers almost all human activities. Until finally reveal a movement toward linguistic studies are multidisplin, one of which is linguistic anthropology.
Entomology linguistics is a branch of linguistics that examines the relationship between language and culture, especially to observe how language is used everyday as a tool in social action. (Lauder, 2005:231) also called ethno linguistic Anthropology examines not only the language of its structure alone but more on functions and their use in the context of socio-cultural situation. The study linguistic anthropology, among others, reviewed the structure and family relationships through kinship terms, the concept of color, the pattern of parenting, or examine how community members communicate with each other in certain situations such as in indigenous ceremonies, and then connect it to the concept of culture.
Example: speech acts pastor '.... With this, I declare you as husband and wife ...' addles an action through language arrives in the community have the authority to establish a pair of wedding became a husband and wife in a legal and legitimate terterima by the community.
In America the melopori science is linguistic anthropology Franz Boas, while in Europe they use the term ethno linguistic (Durante, 1997). Through linguistic anthropology approach, we look at what people are doing with language and speech-speech that is produced; silence and gesture associated with turnout context (Durante, 2001:1).
Dell Hymens was largely responsible for launching the second paradigm that fixed the name "linguistic anthropology" in the 1960s, though he also coined the term "ethnography of speaking" (or "ethnography of communication") to describe the agenda he envisioned for the field. It would involve taking advantage of new developments in technology, including new forms of mechanical recording.
Dell Hymens was largely responsible for launching the second paradigm that fixed the name "linguistic anthropology" in the 1960s, though he also coined the term "ethnography of speaking" (or "ethnography of communication") to describe the agenda he envisioned for the field. It would involve taking advantage of new developments in technology, including new forms of mechanical recording. A new unit of analysis was also introduced by Hymens. Whereas the first paradigm focused on ostensibly distinct "languages" (scare quotes indicate that contemporary linguistic anthropologists treat the concept of "a language" as an ideal construction covering up complexities within and "across" so-called linguistic boundaries), the unit of analysis in the second paradigm was new—the "speech event." (The speech event is an event defined by the speech occurring in it—a lecture, for example—so that a dinner is not a speech event, but a speech situation, a situation in which speech may or may not occur.) Much attention was devoted to speech events in which performers were held accountable for the form of their linguistic performance as such.
Hymes also pioneered a linguistic anthropological approach to ethno poetics. Hymes had hoped to link linguistic anthropology more closely with the mother discipline. The name certainly stresses that the primary identity is with anthropology, whereas "anthropological linguistics" conveys a sense that the primary identity of its practitioners was with linguistics, which is a separate academic discipline on most university campuses today (not in the days of Boas and Sapir). However, Hymes' ambition in a sense backfired; the second paradigm in fact marked a further distancing of the sub discipline from the rest of anthropology.


 Anthropological issues studied via linguistic methods and data
In the third paradigm, which has emerged since the late 1980s, instead of continuing to pursue agendas that come from a discipline alien to anthropology, linguistic anthropologists have systematically addressed themselves to problems posed by the larger discipline of anthropology—but using linguistic data and methods. Popular areas of study in this third paradigm include investigations of social identities, broadly shared ideologies, and the construction and uses of narrative in interaction among individuals and groups.

Areas of interest
Contemporary linguistic anthropology continues research in all three of the paradigms described above. Several areas related to the third paradigm, the study of anthropological issues, are particularly rich areas of study for current linguistic anthropologists.

Identity
A great deal of work in linguistic anthropology investigates questions of sociocultural identity linguistically. Linguistic anthropologist Don Kulick has done this in relation to identity, for example, in a series of settings, first in a village called Gapun in Papua New Guinea. Kulick explored how the use of two languages with and around children in Gapun village—the traditional language (Taiap) not spoken anywhere but in their own village and thus primordially "indexical" of Gapuner identity, and Tok Pisin (the widely circulating official language of New Guinea). (Linguistic anthropologists use "indexical" to mean indicative, though some indexical signs create their indexical meanings on the fly, so to speak. To speak the Taiap language is associated with one identity—not only local but "Backward" and also an identity based on the display of *hed* (personal autonomy). To speak Tok Pisin is to index a modern, Christian (Catholic) identity, based not on *hed* but on *save*, that is an identity linked with the will and the skill to cooperate. In later work, Kulick demonstrates that certain loud speech performances called *um escândalo*, Brazilian travesti (roughly, 'transvestite') sex workers shame clients. The travesti community, the argument goes, ends up at least making a powerful attempt to transcend the shame the larger Brazilian public might try to foist off on them—again, through loud public discourse and other modes of performance.

Socialization
In a series of studies, linguistic anthropologists Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin addressed the important anthropological topic of socialization (the process by which infants, children, and foreigners become members of a community, learning to participate in its culture), using linguistic as well as ethnographic methods.[10] They discovered that the processes of enculturation and socialization do not occur apart from the process of language acquisition, but that children acquire language and culture together in what amounts to an integrated process. Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that baby talk is not universal, that the direction of adaptation (whether the child is made to adapt to the ongoing situation of speech around it or vice versa) was a variable that correlated, for example, with the direction it was held vis-à-vis a caregiver's body. In many societies caregivers hold a child facing outward so as to orient it to a network of kin whom it must learn to recognize early in life.
Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that members of all societies socialize children both to and through the use of language. Ochs and Taylor uncovered how, through naturally occurring stories told during dinners in white middle class households in southern California, both mothers and fathers participated in replicating male dominance (the "father knows best" syndrome) by the distribution of participant roles such as protagonist (often a child but sometimes mother and almost never the father) and "problematizer" (often the father, who raised uncomfortable questions or challenged the competence of the protagonist). When mothers collaborated with children to get their stories told they unwittingly set themselves up to be subject to this process.
Schieffelin's more recent research has uncovered the socializing role of pastors and other fairly new Bosavi converts in the Southern Highlands, Papua New Guinea community she studies. Pastors have introduced new ways of conveying knowledge i.e. new linguistic epistemic markers and new ways of speaking about time. And they have struggled with and largely resisted those parts of the Bible that speak of being able to know the inner states of others (e.g. the gospel of Mark, chapter 2, verses 6-8).

Ideologies
In a third example of the current (third) paradigm, since Roman Jakobson's student, Michael Silverstein opened the way, there has been an efflorescence of work done by linguistic anthropologists on the major anthropological theme of ideologies[15]—in this case "language ideologies", sometimes defined as "shared bodies of commonsense notions about the nature of language in the world."[16] Silverstein has demonstrated that these ideologies are not mere false consciousness but actually influence the evolution of linguistic structures, including the dropping of "thee" and "thou" from everyday English usage.[17] Woolard, in her overview of "code switching", or the systematic practice of alternating linguistic varieties within a conversation or even a single utterance, finds the underlying question anthropologists ask of the practice—Why do they do that?—reflects a dominant linguistic ideology. It is the ideology that people should "really" be monoglot and efficiently targeted toward referential clarity rather than diverting themselves with the messiness of multiple varieties in play at a single time.
Attitudes toward languages such as Spanish and English in the U.S. are certainly informed by linguistic ideologies. This extends to the widespread impression, created by statements such as that by U.S. Senator Lamar Alexander, Republican of Tennessee (in regards to a recently passed measure making English the "official" language of the U.S.), that English is "part of our blood." To Horwitz, this invocation of blood implies that English reflects the deepest vein of the nation's ancestry, i.e., the oldest language spoken in what is now the United States. Such a claim, if made openly, would be doubly absurd, ignoring a) all of the Native American languages severely impacted by the arrival of Europeans, but also b) Spanish, the language of a rather sizable number of European explorers and settlers across the length and breadth of what is now the United States.[19] Thus Alexander is attempting to "naturalize" language and national identity via the metaphor of "blood."
Much research on linguistic ideologies probes subtler influences on language, such as the pull exerted on Tewa — a Kiowa-Tanoan language spoken in certain New Mexico Pueblos as well as on the Hopi Reservation in Arizona — by "kiva speech," discussed in the next section.
Social space
In a final example of this third paradigm, a group of linguistic anthropologists has done very creative work on the idea of social space. Durante published a ground breaking article on Samoan greetings and their use and transformation of social space.[21] Prior to that, Indonesianist Joseph Errington — making use of earlier work by Indonesianists not necessarily concerned with language issues per se—brought linguistic anthropological methods (and semiotic theory) to bear on the notion of the "exemplary center," or the center of political and ritual power from which emanated exemplary behavior.[22] Errington demonstrated how the Javanese *priyayi*, whose ancestors served at the Javanese royal courts, became emissaries, so to speak, long after those courts had ceased to exist, representing throughout Java the highest example of 'refined speech.' The work of Joel Kuipers further develops this theme vis-a-vis the island of Sumba, Indonesia. And, even though it pertains to Tewa Indians in Arizona rather than Indonesians, Paul Kroskrity's argument that speech forms originating in the Tewa kiva (or underground ceremonial space) forms the dominant model for all Tewa speech can be seen as a rather direct parallel.
Silverstein tries to find the maximum theoretical significance and applicability in this idea of exemplary centers. He feels, in fact, that the exemplary center idea is one of linguistic anthropology's three most important findings. He generalizes the notion in the following manner, arguing that "there are wider-scale institutional 'orders of interactionality,' historically contingent yet structured. Within such large-scale, macrosocial orders, in-effect ritual centers of semiosis come to exert a structuring, value-conferring influence on any particular event of discursive interaction with respect to the meanings and significance of the verbal and other semiotic forms used in it. Current approaches to such classic anthropological topics as ritual by linguistic anthropologists emphasize not static linguistic structures but the unfolding in realtime of a "'hypertrophic' set of parallel orders of iconicity and indexicality that seem to cause the ritual to create its own sacred space through what appears, often, to be the magic of textual and nontextual metricalizations, synchronized.
References

Komentar

Postingan Populer